|
August 4-5,
2004 |
8:00-10:00PM, Wednesday, August 4
Union Ballroom - II
Contributed Poster Presenters: Please follow the instructions provided here.
We categorized the Contributed Posters (CP) based on the abstracts. The following themes emerged:
Several posters may lie in more than one category, however for the purposes of organizing this session, we chose what we believed to be the most appropriate category for each poster. Below we have listed the posters by category.
Posters in each category will be located contiguously. A room layout for the Contributed Poster Session indicating the location of each poster will be provided.
Assessment Issues (CP-AI)
The
design and validation of the Colorado Learning Attitudes about Science Survey
Wendy Adams (
Katherine Perkins (katherine.perkins@colorado.edu)
,
Noah Finkelstein
,
Carl Wieman
,
Abstract: The Colorado
Learning Attitudes about Science Survey (CLASS) is a new instrument designed to
measure various facets of student attitudes and beliefs about learning
physics. This instrument extends the
work done by the
E. F. Redish et al, Am J Phys. 66, 212-224
(1998).
Halloun et al, Science and Education 7:6, 553-577 (1998)
B. White et al, presented at the
American Education Research Association.,
Supported by NSF
Evaluating
and Using BEMA (Brief Electricity & Magnetism Assessment)
Lin Ding (
Robert Beichner
,
Ruth Chabay (rwchabay@unity.ncsu.edu)
,
Bruce Sherwood (basherwo@unity.ncsu.edu)
,
Abstract: BEMA is a
comprehensive multiple-choice test designed to assess students' mastery of
fundamental concepts in electricity and magnetism after taking the
calculus-based introductory E&M course [1]. We will present results of
statistical studies that show that BEMA has good reliability, an important
measure for evaluating a test, and we will briefly explain the reasons for
doing such studies. BEMA has been used in previous comparisons of traditional and
reform courses [2]. We plan to use BEMA in further studies in Spring 2004.
*Supported in part by NSF grant
DUE-0320608.
Qualitative Underst
Matter & Interactions II:
Electric & Magnetic Interactions. Ruth Chabay & Bruce Sherwood, Wiley
2002, http://www4.ncsu.edu/~rwchabay/mi
Underst
Elizabeth Gire (egire@physics.ucsd.edu
) ,
Barbara Jones
,
Abstract: Student responses
on the Maryland
Redish, et al., Am. J. Phys. 66, 212-224 (1998).
A
Conceptual Hierarchy of Lunar Phases?
Aaron Hines (
Abstract: According to
cognitive theory, to encourage the development of a scientific underst
Eliciting
and Representing Hybrid Mental Models
Dean Zollman (dzollman@phys.ksu.edu)
,
N.
Abstract: While
constructing their underst
Supported in part by NSF Grant
# 0087788.
Hrepic, Z., D. Zollman, and S.
Rebello. Identifying students' models of sound propagation. in 2002
Development
of an instrument for evaluating anxiety caused by cognitive conflict*
Yeounsoo Kim (
Abstract:
Using a
Q-type assessment instrument to study correlation between teacher attitudes and
student perceptions of physics laboratories
Yuhfen Lin (
Xueli Zou (
Gordon Aubrecht (aubrecht.1@osu.edu)
, The Ohio State University
Abstract: A modified
version of the Laboratory Program Variables Inventory (LPVI),[1] a Q-type instrument originally developed to
assess chemistry laboratories, has been used to study the correlation between
instructor expectations and student descriptions. Careful study of the
correlation among different classes shows that Q-type assessment is an
effective tool for describing course types (as reported in a companion poster).
Here we examine correlations between instructor expectations and student perceptions
among different sections of the same course, as well as differences in student
perceptions among the sections taught by the same instructor. This Q-type
assessment tool may be used to diagnose problems in curriculum development and
instructor education.
M. R. Abraham, 'A descriptive
instrument for use in investigating science laboratories,' Journal of Research
in Science Teaching 19 (2) 155-165 (1982).
Measuring
Conceptual Change in College Students’ Underst
Abstract: Researchers now
know that college students enter the introductory astronomy classroom with
pre-existing mental models of lunar phases.
If rooted deeply enough alternate mental models may actually impair an
individual’s ability to learn a particular concept. To teach the subject successfully,
instructors need to encourage conceptual change. To aid instructors in assessing individuals’
mental models of lunar phases, the Lunar Phases Concept Inventory (LPCI) was
developed. This twenty-item multiple-choice inventory was designed to advantage
of the innovative model analysis theory.
By using this theory in combination with the LPCI, an instructor can
determine the probabilities of their class utilizing different mental models,
as well as how consistently said mental models are utilized. To assess conceptual change, an instructor
can use the LPCI to assess student’s mental models both before and after
instruction. As an example of this
technique, analysis of pre- and post-test LPCI results will be reported.
Student
“Splits” Between Intuition and Scientist Answers*
Timothy McCaskey (
Abstract: Previous work
showed that, on FCI items, students indicate that the answer they “really
believe” often differs from the answer they think a scientist would give
[1]. However, interviews revealed that
these “splits” could not be cleanly interpreted: sometimes they corresponded to a student’s
intuition, sometimes not [2]. For this
reason, and because intuition splits are epistemologically interesting in their
own right, we modified the FCI task.
Students now indicate their 'scientist answer' and the answer that
“makes the most intuitive sense” to them.
New interviews established that the modified task suffers from fewer
interpretive difficulties. In addition,
evidence suggests that students reconcile concepts like
* Supported by NSF grant
#REC-0087519.
T. McCaskey et al., 127th
AAPT National Meeting (2003).
T. McCaskey et al., 128th AAPT
National Meeting (2004).
A Survey
to Investigate Student Underst
Jeffrey Morgan (
Michael Wittmann (michael.
Abstract: Initial
interviews on quantum tunneling with undergraduate physics majors[1] have
revealed that a significant percentage of students (a) believe energy is lost
in tunneling and (b) have difficulty sketching and interpreting the wave
function in the region of a potential barrier, corroborating the findings of
Bao[2] and others[3]. We have used these
results to construct a survey designed to probe student conceptual underst
Correlating
student attitudes with student learning using the Colorado Learning Attitudes
about Science Survey
Katherine Perkins (
Wendy Adams (Wendy.Adams@colorado.edu)
,
Steven Pollock
,
Carl Wieman
,
Noah Finkelstein
,
Abstract: A number of
instruments have been designed to probe the “hidden curriculum”[1], examining
the variety of attitudes, beliefs, expectations, and epistemological frames
taught in our introductory physics courses.
Using a newly developed instrument – the CLASS[2] – we examine the
relationship among students’ attitudes and beliefs, their shifts over the
course of a semester, and other educational outcomes, such as conceptual
learning and student retention. We
report results from surveys of 2400 students in a variety of courses, including
several designed to promote favorable student attitudes. We find positive correlations between
particular student attitudes and conceptual learning gains, and between student
retention and favorable attitudes and beliefs in select categories. We also note the influence of teaching
practices on student attitudes.
E. F. Redish, Teaching
W. Adams et al., The design and
validation of the Colorado Learning Attitudes about Science Survey, PERC 2004.
Student
underst
Paul Reilly (
Abstract: We are
investigating student difficulties and designing tutorials related to Gauss' law in introductory calculus-based
courses. Our investigation includes
interviews with individual students, development and administration of free-response pre-/post-tests, and development
of a conceptual multiple-choice
test. Results of our investigation will
be discussed.
From
students’ perspectives: A Q-type assessment instrument*
Xueli Zou (
Yuhfen Lin
, The
Gordon Aubrecht
, The
Abstract: A Q-type
instrument, the Laboratory Program Variables Inventory (LPVI)**, has been used
to assess three possible different introductory physics laboratories: an
investigative science learning laboratory at California State University, Chico
(CSUC),
*Supported in part by NSF DUE #
0242845 and #0088906.
**M. R. Abraham, “A descriptive
instrument for use in investigating science laboratories,” Journal of Research
in Science Teaching 19 (2) 155-165
Difficulties & Misconceptions (CP-DM)
Rate of Change
and Electric Potential
Rhett Allain (
Robert Beichner (beichner@ncsu.edu)
,
Abstract: This project aims
to investigate a possible underlying cause to student difficulties relating
change of electric potential to electric field.
A likely source of difficulties is the lack of students' underst
Diminishing
Forces – Implications of a Misconception
Abstract: Evidence is
presented to suggest a misconception concerning motion of an object when acted
upon by a force which decreases with distance. This evidence was collected
during interviews of several above average calculus-based physics
students. The students stated that the
motion of an object would slow, even stop, if the force on decreased based upon
its distance such as Coulomb’s Law. This
may not be surprising until viewed it in the light that many of these students
didn’t reveal this impetus or Aristotelian notion except with diminishing
forces.
Assessing
student underst
Abstract: Students often
confuse wave amplitude and intensity.
They state that they see a wave peak at points of maximum
interference. In order to better assess
this confusion, a waves questionnaire was given to 259 students during the 3rd
quarter introductory calculus-based physics class at the Ohio State University
just after all lecture instruction regarding wave interference and diffraction
had been completed. Although further
study will be necessary to understand many of the student responses, several
misconceptions were evident from the results.
Among these are that a large number of students believe that
interference is purely destructive interference; many think that the eye can
distinguish wave peaks and troughs; and most students think the wave peaks are
points of highest intensity. The
detailed findings from the questionnaire will be reported in this poster.
Student
difficulties with graphical representation of vector products: crossing and
dotting beyond t’s and i’s*
Warren Christensen (
Ngoc-Loan Nguyen (nguyenn@iastate.edu)
,
David Meltzer (
Abstract: Recent research
[1-3] has shown that students in introductory physics courses (both algebra-
and calculus-based) have significant difficulty with the graphical
representation of vectors. In order to
understand concepts such as work, torque, and magnetic force on a charged
particle, students must have a coherent underst
*Supported in part by NSF REC
#0206683
R.D. Knight, Phys. Teach. 33,
74 (1995)
L.G. Ortiz, P.R.L. Heron,
P.S. Shaffer, and L.C. McDermott, AAPT Announcer 31(4), 103 (2001)
N-L Nguyen and D.E. Meltzer,
Am. J. Phys. 71, 630 (2003)
Identifying
student concepts of “gravity”
Roger
E. Feeley (
John
R. Thompson
,
Michael
C. Wittmann
,
Abstract: We have
investigated student concepts of “gravity” among non-science majors,
pre-service K-12 teachers, and high school students. Both interview and survey questions were
developed or modified from those in the literature [1, 2, 3]. Students were questioned on their reasoning
about the behavior of objects on the surface of a planetary body (e.g., the
Earth or the moon) and the causes of this behavior. Results will be presented indicating that
the survey successfully elicited student difficulties with various aspects of
gravity, including the tendency to attribute gravity to the presence of an
atmosphere, and to dissociate the concepts of gravity and weight.
Student
underst
Michael Loverude (
Abstract: We have been
investigating student underst
Student
underst
Katherine Menchen (
Abstract: Our ongoing
research involves exploring student underst
K. VP. Menchen and
J. R. Thompson, “Preservice teacher underst
Contrasts
in student underst
Cristian Raduta (
Gordon Aubrecht (
Abstract: We administered a
survey on electricity and magnetism to two populations of students: one from
General Interest (CP-GI)
Design-Based
Research: A Primer for
Richard Hake (
Abstract: Some prominent
education researchers now work in Pasteur’s interdisciplinary, use-inspired,
basic-research quadrant doing what they call 'Design-Based Research' (DBR).
After quoting descriptions of DBR by a few of its advocates, I discuss the
insularity that has hidden DBR’s from PER’s
and PER’s from DBR’s. I then attempt to make the case that: (a) some PER
is also DBR; (b) randomized control trials (RCT’s) - not generally a part of
DBR - are not the 'gold standard' of educational research, as hailed by the
U.S. Dept. of Education; (c) DBR might develop into a force sufficient to
accelerate even the ponderous educational system: (d) the pre/post test
movement, generally ignored by the education community, could be a major
component of that reforming force; and (e) non-classical analyses of tests
heretofore used primarily for pre/post testing might assist the underst
Submitted to the
Are
physics graduate students’ beliefs about teaching and learning consonant?
Yuhfen Lin (
Gordon Aubrecht (aubrecht.1@osu.edu)
, The Ohio State University
Abstract: Most physics graduate
students teach as recitation or lab instructors in introductory physics courses
at the same time they take graduate level courses. These students may or may
not apply the same standards they expect in the courses they take to their own
classes. We investigate whether they apply similar or distinct standards to
these courses. This talk focuses on aspects of both teaching of and learning by
OSU
The
Journal of Research in Science Teaching
Abstract: As of January
2005, the Journal of Research in Science Teaching and Learning (JRST) will be
under new editorship. The new editors
would particularly like to invite the discipline-based education research
community to submit articles for publication in JRST. Although the journal is read by both
researchers and practitioners, the focus is on research progress rather than
research-to-practice. Studies by the American Educational Research Journal and
the Educational Researcher for the American Educational Research Association
identified JRST as one of the top research journals in science education. Many
types of scholarly manuscripts about research on science teaching and learning
are within JRST's domain, including but not limited to: investigations,
employing experimental, qualitative, ethnographic, historical, survey,
philosophical, or case study research approaches; position papers; policy
perspectives; and critical reviews of the literature. After January, all
submissions and correspondence will be electronic, though the journal will
continue to be published on paper.
Instructional Practices (CP-IP)
Can
inquiry experiences in physics class change students’ preconceptions about
teaching?
Gordont Aubrecht (
Abstract: In teaching inquiry
classes in physics, we ask students to reflect on their learning in journals.
One of the journal questions deals with student expectations of transfer of the
inquiry techniques used in our class into their own classrooms when they become
teachers themselves. We report on students’ answers to this question over our
decade-long experience in running inquiry courses, which gives insight into how
much or how little the students think the techniques are worth to themselves as
both students and prospective teachers.
Use of a
hands-on lab exam to investigate how physics students transfer knowledge from
lecture to the laboratory
Duane Deardorff (
Abstract: For the past 4
years, an individual, hands-on lab exam has been administered to our
introductory physics students. As
advertised to the students in their lab manual, the purpose of this exam is to
assess each student's ability to make accurate measurements with typical
physics laboratory instruments, analyze and interpret empirical data, apply
fundamental physics principles, design simple experiments, evaluate results,
analyze measurement errors, and clearly communicate findings. These exams have generated a rich set of data
that can be used to help answer a variety of research questions about how
students make and analyze measurements.
Selected findings from this investigation will focus on students'
abilities and difficulties in transferring knowledge from the lecture to the
laboratory setting.
Virtual
Reality Experiments in Introductory
Stephen Stonebraker
, The
Abstract: Physicists
consider laboratories to be a vital part of any introductory course, yet
students consistently rate them as having low value. The Ohio State University (OSU)
Helping
preservice teachers implement and assess research-based instruction in K-12
classrooms
Lezlie
S. DeWater (
Donna Messina (messina@phys.washington.edu)
,
MacKenzie Stetzer (stetzer@phys.washington.edu)
,
Abstract: The
Can
Computer Simulations Replace Real Equipment in Undergraduate Laboratories?
Noah Finkelstein (Noah.Finkelstein@colorado.edu)
,
Katherine Perkins (
Wendy Adams (Wendy.Adams@colorado.edu)
,
Abstract: This poster
examines the effects of substituting computer simulations in place of real
laboratory equipment in the second semester of a large-scale introductory
physics course. The direct current (DC)
circuit laboratory was modified to compare the effects of using computer
simulations with the effects of using real light bulbs, meters and wires. Three groups of students, those who used real
equipment, those who used computer simulations, and those who had no lab
experience, were compared in terms of their mastery of physics concepts and
skills with real equipment. Note the
complete author list: N. D. Finkelstein, K. K. Perkins, W. Adams, P. Kohl, and
N. Podolefsky
Learning
Danielle Harlow (
Valerie Otero (Valerie.Otero@colorado.edu)
,
Abstract: This study
provides evidence to support the claim that prospective elementary teachers can
deepen their underst
Evaluating
Options for Combating Post-Exam Syndrome
Kathleen Harper (
Matt Finnerty
,
Robert
W. Brown
,
Abstract: A previous talk
described exam correction assignments to assist students in using midterms as a
learning tool.1 Preliminary results
suggested that student learning resulted.
Some new studies strive to answer several subsequent questions: Does learning result from the nature of the
assignment, or from the students simply putting in additional time with the
material? Does working additional
problems on a topic post-exam impact student learning of the material? Does the way in which instructors present
and/or explain the exam-correction steps have an impact on learning? In the primary study, four parallel sections
of a large course, after receiving back their graded exams, either did exam
corrections, worked problems similar to the exam, or worked problems on new
material. All took a follow-up test on
the same topics as the initial test. The
results from this study, and their implications for the issues described above,
will be discussed.
K. A. Harper & R. W. Brown, “A
Treatment for Post-Exam Syndrome,” 128th AAPT National Meeting (2004).
Teaching,
Learning and PER: Views from Mainstream Faculty
Charles Henderson (
Melissa Dancy (dancy@email.uncc.edu)
,
Abstract: Anecdotal
evidence suggests that findings of educational research and resulting curricula
are, at best, only marginally incorporated into introductory physics courses.
We are working on a long-term project to provide the PER community with
information that will facilitate the incorporation of research-based strategies
and materials into mainstream physics instruction. In this talk, we will report
on the pilot phase of this project which involved interviews with well
respected and thoughtful senior
Electrostatic & Magnetism TIPERs*
Curtis Hieggelke (
David Maloney (
Thomas O'Kuma (tokuma@lee.edu)
,
Abstract: This paper will
illustrate materials from a collection of new instructional materials for the
topics and concepts in electrostatics and magnetism. These materials can be
used as classroom materials, quizzes or exam questions, or homework. These
materials employ various TIPER (Tasks Inspired by
*Supported in part by CCLI grants
# 9952735 and 0125831 from the Division of Undergraduate Education of the
National Science Foundation
Using The
Schema Conceptual Tool To Promote Student Underst
Brant Hinrichs (
Abstract: The Modeling
Instruction program at
Representational
Format, Student Choice, and Problem Solving in
Noah Finkelstein (
Abstract: Student
problem-solving ability appears to be tied to the representational format of
the problem (mathematical, pictorial, graphical, verbal). In a study of a 367-student algebra-based
physics class, we examine student problem solving ability on quizzes involving
four different representational formats, with problems as close to isomorphic
as possible. In addition, we examine
students’ capacity for representational self-assessment by giving follow-up
quizzes in which they can choose between various problem formats, and look for
factors that may influence their ability or choices. As a control, part of the class was assigned
a follow-up quiz with a random format, allowing a comparison with the group
provided with a choice. We find that
there are statistically significant performance differences between isomorphic
problems. We also find that allowing
students to choose which representational form they use increases student
performance under some circumstances, and reduces it in others.
In class
polling: An instant feedback of students learning mode
Pengfei Li (
Neville Reay (reay@mps.ohio-state.edu)
, The Ohio State University
Abstract: At The Ohio State
University, Voting Machine (VM), an in-class polling system was used as an
effective lecture instrument to teach students in an introductory
electromagnetism class. A sequence of three questions (basic, intensive,
extensive) was chosen as a useful tool to trigger the student into learning
mode. In this talk, we will discuss the methods and the results of our
numerical analysis and examples of student behavior patterns extracted from
such analysis.
Attitudes
of General Science Students Towards
Learning Science and the Nature of Science
Shabbir Mian (smain@mcdaniel.edu)
,
Vasilis Pagonis (vpagonis@mcdaniel.edu)
,
Abstract: We investigated
general science students’ attitudes regarding the acquisition of scientific
knowledge and the nature of science itself, by administering a 32-item survey
combining and exp
See
http://www2.physics.umd.edu/~elby/EBAPS/home.htm and
http://www.physics.umd.edu/rgroups/ripe/perg/expects/mpex.htm
Funded by the NSF and
Gender
Equity Variations in a Large Active-Learning Introductory
Mark McKinnon (
Abstract: At the University
of California at Davis, the introductory physics course for life science majors
has been employing an active-learning approach since 1996. Currently, more than
1400 students begin this course each year.
One of the surprising developments has been the significant improvement
in gender equity over the traditional physics sequence. We are now focusing more attention on
utilizing student performance to quantitatively explore a broad range of issues
related to active-learning approaches and the role that they play in gender
equity. In class, small groups of five
to six students team to complete activities designed to elicit discussion about
physical concepts. As the activities
were annually modified, opinions differ on the effectiveness of each variation.
I used two variations of laboratory activities, one less formulaic than the
other. I will present findings of the
relative gender equity as measured by quiz performance of these variations.
Transfer
of Teaching: an Experiment of Opportunity
Robert
A. Morse (
Abstract: To what extent
can an experienced teacher’s interactive engagement physics curriculum,
developed over many years, be taken over by a teacher with only a few year’s
experience? A sabbatical leave for the first author provided an opportunity to
try the experiment. The first author gave course materials and daily plan book
used for introductory physics and AP
Helping
students learn to design experiments in a large-enrollment introductory
laboratory course*
Sahana Murthy (
Abstract: The
* Supported in part by NSF grant
#DUE-0241078.
Etkina, E. (2004).
'Developing and Assessing scientific abilities in an introductory physics
course', AAPT Announcer, Vol. 33 (4), p. 85 (2004).
No single
cause: learning gains, student attitudes, and the impacts of multiple effective
reforms in a large lecture course.
Steven Pollock (
Abstract: We examine the
effects of, and interplay among, several proven research-based reforms implemented
in an introductory large enrollment (500+) calculus based physics course. These
interventions included Peer Instruction with student response system in
lecture[1], Tutorials[2] with trained undergraduate learning assistants in
recitations, and personalized computer assignments[3]. We took extensive survey
data throughout the semester along with validated pre/post content- and
attitude-surveys, and long answer pre/post content questions designed to assess
learning gains and near transfer, to investigate complementary effects of these
multiple reforms, and to begin to understand which features are necessary and
effective for high fidelity replication. Our median normalized gain was 0.67 on
the FCI , 0.76 on the FMCE, yet we find we cannot uniquely associate these
gains with any individual (isolated) course components. We see no decline in
self-reported student attitudes, but do find that attitudes and attitude shifts
both correlate positively with conceptual learning gains.
Peer Instruction, E. Mazur
Tutorials in Introductory
Work supported by
Pew/Carnegie, NSF, and APS PhysTec
Teacher
and Curriculum Factors that Influence Middle School Students' Sense-Making
Discussions of Force/Motion
Cody S
Abstract: This study
investigated small-group discussions in an inquiry-based middle school science
classroom. The purpose of the study was to determine the teacher and curriculum
factors that provide support (or not) for students' sense-making discussions.
To do this, two student groups were videotaped as they participated in
force/motion activities. Analysis revealed that sense-making discussion was
influenced by teacher adherence (or not) to the curriculum philosophy, activity
content, teacher and curricular guidance for the continuing evolution of
student ideas, and other teacher and curriculum factors.
The Role
of Evaluation Abilities in Student Learning & Performance
Aaron Warren (
Alan Van Heuvelen (Alanvan@physics.rutgers.edu)
, Rutgers University
Abstract: This poster
presents research which is part of a larger initiative by the Rutgers University
PAER group to help students develop scientific thinking abilities. In particular, we focus on developing
abilities which are necessary for students to critically evaluate information. Such information can include proposed problem
solutions, conceptual statements, experiment designs, and experiment
reports. To develop and assess
evaluative strategies among students, we are creating and testing a wide range
of activities. This poster presents some
examples of these activities, outlines several types of evaluative strategies
students can use, and examines some preliminary results regarding the role
various evaluative strategies play in student learning and performance.
Student
Participation in Normative Behaviors in a
Benjamin Williams (
Abstract: The
Modeling Student Thinking (CP-MT)
Generated
Analogies as Assertions of Categorization
Leslie Atkins (atkins@umd.edu)
, University of Maryland
Abstract: One could
consider the appropriate use of analogy to be the epitome of transfer; indeed,
research in transfer frequently concerns whether or not an analogy is mapped
onto the desired target (e.g., Gick and Holyoak, 1980). Underst
Active
versus passive learning
Florin Bocaneala (
Abstract: Almost everybody
agrees that the students who are actively involved in structuring their study
environment, who investigate their study matter by selecting and formulating
their own questions, perform better. Is this circumstantial correlation? What
is the dynamics behind increased performance and active involvement in
learning? In order to address this questions, the authors present herein a
theoretical model that allows the comparison between the active and passive
learning styles.
Concerning
Scientific Discourse about Heat
David Brookes (
George Horton
Alan Van Heuvelen
, Rutgers University
Abstract: We aim to examine
communication in physics from a linguistic perspective and suggest a
theoretical viewpoint which may enable
us to explain and understand many physics students’ alternative conceptions. We
present evidence, in the context of the
concept of heat, that physicists seem to speak and write about physical systems
with a set of one or more systematic
metaphors. These metaphors are well understood in their community. We argue
that physics students appear to be prone
to misinterpreting and overextending the same metaphors and that these
misinterpretations exhibit themselves as
students’ alternative conceptions or misconceptions. A detailed analysis
of physicists’ discourse about heat will be used to present evidence of a possible connection
between students’ alternative conceptions and the possibility that they are misinterpreting the metaphorical language
that they read and hear.
Introductory
College Students’ Explanations of Friction and Related Phenomena at the
Microscopic Level
N.
Abstract: Introductory
college physics students’ explanations of friction and lubrication were
investigated by conducting semi-structured clinical interviews. Interview
questions were constructed in a way that students were led to explain phenomena
that they observed at the atomic level. Analysis of data showed that students
were able to come up with their own explanations of what is happening at the
atomic scale when surfaces come into contact, although these explanations are
not necessarily scientifically correct.
It was apparent that students tended to explain phenomena at the atomic
level by using attributes of macroscopic objects (e.g. by visualizing atoms as
balls students tended to associate attributes of real balls to that of atoms).
Results of the study will serve as a basis to design teaching interviews to
help students construct more scientifically correct microscopic models of
friction and lubrication.
Evidence
of Transfer in Interview Data
Paula Engelhardt (engelhar@phys.ksu.edu)
, Kansas State University
Abstract: Transfer is the
ability to utilize what one has learned previously in new situations. This
paper will explore evidence of transfer by one student during the course of a
single interview. The transcript will be
analyzed from two perspectives: the actor-oriented model of transfer developed
by Joanne Lobato and the Kansas State University
Model
What you Preach: Explicitly Articulated Interactions for Transfer of Concepts
by
Cathy Ezrailson (
Abstract: This study used
both quantitative and qualitative methods to gain an underst
This study synthesized active
engagement strategies based on definitive prior research, e.g.: Heller, Keith
& Anderson, 1992; MacIsaac & Falconer, 2002; Minstrell, 2001; Hake,
1998, and many others.
Transfer
between Paired Problems
N.
Abstract: Student reasoning
was originally thought to be fairly stable.
It has now become clear that students’ ideas are not nearly as stable as
originally thought. The question now has
become just how unstable are students’ ideas and what things can influence
these ideas. This paper will cover a
small portion of a larger study designed to address this question. An interview over basic mechanics questions
will be used to show how the questions themselves influence the student’s
answer to the questions. Based on this
transcript and other data collected during the study, students’ ideas appear to
be influenced not only by their experiences and the context presented in the
question, but also by the context of the question. This analysis was done based
on a new model of transfer called the actor-oriented transfer model developed
by Joanne Lobato. This new model will
also be discussed in the paper.
Transfer:
the advantage of simple symbols
Andrew Heckler (
Jennifer Kaminski (kaminski.16@osu.edu)
, The Ohio State University
Vladimir Sloutsky (VSloutsky@hec.ohio-state.edu)
, The Ohio State University
Abstract: One of the goals
of successful learning is transfer, or the ability to apply acquired knowledge
outside the learning situation. However,
spontaneous transfer is notoriously difficult to achieve even for relatively
simple knowledge. One important issue is
whether it is better to learn concrete or abstract knowledge first. This
research argues that transfer of learning across domains can be facilitated
when knowledge is expressed in an abstract, generic form. In two experiments, undergraduate students
learned two isomorphic domains, based on the same algebraic group. The 'math'
domain was expressed in a more abstract, generic form, whereas the 'science'
domain was expressed in a more concrete form.
In both experiments, transfer from more abstract to more concrete was
greater than the reverse. In addition,
Experiment 2 indicated that the use of concrete symbols may hinder
learning. This research supports the
point of view that learning mathematics facilitates learning science. It argues
that while there may be learning benefits in using concrete materials for
instruction, the learning costs are substantial, thus suggesting the need for a
radical rethinking of how mathematics and science is taught.
Sample
Exams and Transfer in Introductory Mechanics
Carol Koleci (
Abstract: We report on a
continuing study at Worcester Polytechnic Institute concerning the use of
sample exams to promote transfer in introductory mechanics. Do sample exam problems have to be easier or
more difficult than the actual exam questions in order for students to
demonstrate improved conceptual underst
Alternative
conceptions, memory, & mental model in physics education
Gyoungho Lee (
Jiyeon Park
Yeonsoo Kim
Abstract: There are two
somewhat independent research traditions, which converge to suggest a form of
knowledge representation: alternative conceptions and mental model. However we
have little literature that explains what they are different from each other
and from memory. This study tried to describe theoretical issues with some
thoughts about how cognitive science and science education approaches can be
best synthesized in order to approach these questions.
Investigating
Students' Knowledge of Particle Structure of Matter in Different Cultures
Cui Lili (lili@phys.ksu.edu)
, Kansas State University
Dean Zollman (dzollman@phys.ksu.edu)
, Kansas State University
N.
Abstract: This study is in
the early stages of an investigation of students’ models of the structure of
matter. Initially, we will compare results for students in three different
countries. We will administer a questionnaire, developed by Silke
Melkelskis-Seifert in Germany, which includes Likert scale and open-ended
questions. This questionnaire focuses on underst
Student
Descriptions of Refraction and Optical Fibers
Fran Mateycik (
DJ Wagner (
JJ Rivera
Sybillyn Jennings (jennis@sage.edu)
, Sage College
Abstract: This paper
reports our research into how students describe and think about optical fibers
and the physical phenomena of refraction and total internal reflection (TIR)
basic to their operation. The study was
conducted as part of the improvement and expansion of web-based materials for
an innovative Rensselaer introductory physics course [1] which examines the
physics underlying information technology.
As we developed the prototype module, we examined students' underst
Transfer
of Learning from Trigonometry to
Darryl Ozimek (djozimek@phys.ksu.edu)
, Kansas State University
Paula
V. Engelhardt (engelhar@phys.ksu.edu)
, Kansas State University
N.
Abstract: We investigated
students’ learning, retention, and transfer from a trigonometry course to an
algebra–based physics course. A
multiple–choice survey was administered as a pre–instruction and
post–instruction assessment. The survey
consisted of questions pairs, abstract (mathematics) and conceptual (physics)
questions at three hierarchical levels of thinking. Three semi–structured interviews used
graduated–prompting to determine the ease at which students transfer what they
have learned from mathematics (abstract) questions to similar physics
(contextual) questions. Results indicate
that students’ thinking of trigonometric concepts occurs at different
levels. Concepts at lower levels are
retained and transferred to a greater degree than higher level concepts. Transfer was assessed from the perspectives
of both the traditional as well as the contemporary models of transfer. This study has implications for instruction
of both trigonometry and physics as well as suggestions for improving transfer
of learning from one area to another.
Analogical
Scaffolding of Abstract Ideas in
Noah Podolefsky (
Wendy Adams (
Noah Finkelstein (
Abstract: Physicists
commonly use analogies to ground their underst
Reiner M,Slotta J,Chi
M,Resnick L(2000).Naïve
Supported by the Kavli Foundation
and the NSF.
Learning
and knowledge transfer between physics problems
David Pritchard (
Elsa-Sofia Morote
, Massachusetts Institute of Technology
Rasil Warnakulasooriya
,
Massachusetts Institute of Technology
Abstract: We compare two
equally skilled groups who solve pairs of tutorial and related problems in
introductory mechanics at MIT using the web-based tutor, myCyberTutor. The two
groups solve the problems in reverse order with respect to each other. The
group that solves the tutorials first experiences twice as large a reduction in
difficulty per unit of time spent on the tutorial problem as the other group
experiences on the tutorial due to solving the related problem first. The time
for completion graphs provides confirmatory analysis-preparatory problems
reduce the time necessary to solve the following problem. Further evidence is
obtained by analyzing the use of hints between the two related problems. The
group which solves a problem in a given problem-pair first requests more hints
than the group which solves it second and benefits from these hints in
answering the second problem in the pair. We conclude that learning and
knowledge transfer is better facilitated through a tutorial-first approach than
by a problem-first approach. We find evidence of schema acquisition and support
the cognitive theory of feedback as a form of information that helps students
in physics learning.
Multiple
Representations: A Quantitative Study on
Students Use of Free-Body Diagrams in Large Lecture Classes.*
David Rosengrant (
Alan Van Heuvelen (alanvan@physics.rutgers.edu)
, Rutgers University
Abstract: The Rutgers PAER
group is working to help students develop various scientific abilities. One of the abilities is to create, understand
and learn to use for a qualitative reasoning and problem solving different
representations of physical processes such as pictorial representations, motion
diagrams, free body diagrams, and energy bar charts.
Student
Learning of Quantum Mechanics
Abstract: Quantum physics
is an abstract topic that not only deals with the inaccessible venues and
concepts of the microscopic world, but also requires a certain degree of
mathematical skill. In this study, we
aim to determine the most important skills and concepts that can enhance
students’ performance in a quantum mechanics class. We would like to know, for example, if a
relationship exists between particular math skills and underst
The
Structure of Intermediate Mechanics Students'
Eleanor
C Sayre (
Michael
C Wittmann (
Abstract: As part of
ongoing research into cognitive processes and student thought, we investigate
the structure of physics and mathematics intuitions in intermediate mechanics
students. Students compared various damped and undamped harmonic motions using
both differential equations and verbal descriptions of physical systems. We
present evidence from a reformed sophomore-level mechanics class which contains
both tutorial [1] and lecture components. Preliminary data suggest that mathematics
and physics intuitions, even in intermediate students, are poorly linked and
occasionally lead to conflicting predictions.
Bradley S. Ambrose, 'Investigating
student underst
Students’
conceptions about probability in a double-slit experiment for electrons and
potential well problems
Pornrat Wattanakasiwich (
Kenneth Krane (kranek@physics.orst.edu)
, Oregon State University
Abstract: Underst
What
changes occur during conceptual change?
Michael Wittmann (
Abstract: In their 1998
paper, diSessa & Sherin [1] answer the question 'what changes in conceptual
change?' by introducing the idea of coordination classes. These consist of
readout strategies for gathering information about a situation and causal nets
of activated resources that generate thinking in a situation. Several types of
conceptual change have been described in the literature (e.g., incremental,
wholesale, cascade, dual construction [2]). All can be described using
coordination classes. In this poster, I illustrate each form of conceptual
change schematically and suggest other possibile forms of conceptual change
that may play a role in student learning of physics.
diSessa, A. A., and Sherin,
B. L. (1998). What changes in conceptual change. International Journal of
Science Education, 20(10), 1155-1191.
Demastes, S., Good, R. ,
Peebles, P. (1996). Patterns of conceptual change in evolution. Journal of
Research in Science Teaching, 33(4), 407-431.
Edit Yerushalmi (
Bat
Sheva Eylon (nteylon@wisemail.weizmann.ac.il)
, Weizmann Institute
Rachel Seggev (ntseggev@wisemail.weizmann.ac.il)
, Weizmann Institute
Abstract: Transfer is a
central goal for problem solving, though it spans from transferring procedures
within a finite set of similar “end of chapter problems” to developing
independent learning skills. Students' perceptions regarding knowledge and
learning are an important factor in achieving transfer goals. Teachers that
renew their instruction methods to achieve these goals have to become more
attentive to such students' perceptions. We conducted a cooperative inquiry
workshop to support teachers that renew their instruction to develop students'
independent learning skills in physics problems solving. In this paper, we describe how teachers raise
issues related to students’ perceptions of knowledge and learning, as well as
the development of a questionnaire examining these issues, designed by both the
teachers and the researchers. We present findings from the analysis of the
questionnaire and look at how the teachers’ thinking was reformulated as a
result of the process to influence their instruction.
Karen Cummings (cummingsk2
Edward Grillo , Weizmann Institute
Abstract:
In this paper we explore students’ pre-instruction knowledge of conceptual and
procedural pieces of knowledge that we believe are prerequisite to one’s ability
to generate correct light ray diagrams. We do so within the domain of image
formation by a plane mirror. In addition, we follow students as they transition
between various states of understanding using “dynamic assessment” techniques.
That is, we probe student understanding as it develops- throughout
instruction-rather than only pre- and post-instruction. The research population
is students in an algebra-based, introductory physics course at a medium-sized,
urban, public university.
Technology in Research & Teaching (CP-TR)
A
Web-based Tool for the Analysis of Concept Inventory Data
Joseph Beuckman (
Abstract: Computing
technology now makes possible previously impractical methods of analyzing
student assessment data beyond the traditional 'total average score'
approach. Our new, web-based tool will
allow researchers in any location to upload their data and quickly download a
complete analysis report. Analyses
eventually included with this tool will be basic test statistics, Model
Analysis Theory results, concept structure analysis, Traditional Item Analysis,
Concentration Item Analysis, pre and post test comparison, including the
calculations of gains, normalized change and effect size. The tool currently
analyzes data from the Lunar Phases Concept Inventory (LPCI). It will be expanded to analyze data from
other commonly utilized concept inventories in the PER community and,
eventually, from user-designed and uploaded conceptual domains and
inventories. In this poster, we will
discuss the development of this analysis tool, as well as present our results
to date. Instructors and researchers are
encouraged to use the latest version of the analysis tool via our website.
Feedback
with web-based homework and PADs
Scott Bonham (
Abstract: The
Computerized
interactive problem-solving coaches
Leon Hsu (
Abstract: Computers can
play an important role in physics instruction by coaching students to develop
good problem-solving skills. Building on
previous research on the teaching of
problem solving and on computer-student interactions, we are designing computer
tutorials that provide students with guided practice in solving problems. We present a prototype of a tutorial along
with students’ reactions to it and discuss some preliminary results regarding
the transfer of problem-solving skills from the computer tutorials to
pencil-and-paper.
Student
difficulties with computer modeling: using protocol data to revise instruction
Matthew Kohlmyer (
Ruth Chabay (rwchabay@unity.ncsu.edu)
, North Carolina State University
Bruce Sherwood (basherwo@unity.ncsu.edu)
, North Carolina State University
Abstract: Computer
modeling, an important skill in modern physics research, is emphasized by the
Matter & Interactions (1) introductory physics curriculum. Students in this curriculum write computer
programs that model a wide variety of physical systems using an iterative
application of fundamental physics principles.
In order for students to be able to do this successfully, instructors
must know the difficulties students have in learning computer modeling. Based on results from a think-aloud protocol study in Spring 2003 at NC State,
instructional materials were designed
and implemented at the beginning of the Fall 2003 semester. A second think-aloud protocol was then
conducted to search for qualitative differences and similarities in students'
difficulties and reasoning processes while engaged in computer modeling. Results of this study and how they affect the
future instructional revision cycle for Matter & Interactions will be
presented.
Toward an
effective use of voting machines in physics lectures
Neville Reay (reay@mps.ohio-state.edu)
, The Ohio State University
Pengfei Li (li.427@osu.edu)
, The Ohio State University
Rasil Warnakulasooriya (rasil@MIT.EDU)
, Massachusetts Institute of Technology
Abstract: “Voting Machines”
(VM) is a generic name for wireless-keypad in-class polling systems used by
students to answer multiple-choice questions during lectures. Use of VM with
carefully designed sequences of multiple-choice questions and instantaneous
voting summaries improved classroom dynamics and rapidly guided students
through a step-by-step process of assimilating concepts in the electricity and magnetism quarter of a
year-long beginning physics course.
Raw and analyzed results, class surveys and sample question sequences
will be presented for two lecture sections, one in which students voted in groups
and the other in which they voted as individuals
A Study
of Student Use of an Online Message Board in an Introductory
Wenjuan Song (
Taha Mzoughi (mzoughi@ra.msstate.edu)
, Mississippi State University
Anastasia Elder (aelder@ra.msstate.edu)
, Mississippi State University
Abstract: We investigated
the use of an online message board by students in an Introductory
Calculus-based physics course. The paper will describe the student population,
the format of the course taught, the type of messages posted by the students
and the message board used. The study includes correlational analysis between
the number of the various kinds of messages posted by the students, and the
student performance in homework, tests and overall course grade. Further data
provides insight on student beliefs about the use of message board.
Using
Electronic Interviews to Explore Student Underst
DJ Wagner (
JJ Rivera
Fran Mateycik (
Sybillyn Jennings (jennis@sage.edu)
, Sage College
Abstract: This paper
reports on methods used to probe student underst
Time for
completion curves for physics problems
Rasil Warnakulasooriya (
David Pritchard
, Massachusetts Institute of Technology
Abstract: Using
myCyberTutor, a web-based homework tutor, we study how long it takes students
to complete a given physics problem completely and correctly. We identify three
major groups of students in completing a given problem. The students who were
able to solve the problem quickly (< 2.5 min), we hypothesize are able to
solve it through some insight or having worked it out previously. The major
group of students who completes the problem (in 2.5 min to 2 hours) often uses
hints and feedback. The third group takes over several hours, generally days.
We hypothesize that they obtain help outside myCyberTutor. The middle part of
the graphs (typically 2 min to 2 hours) of the fraction of students completing
a given problem as a function of logarithmic time yields sigmoid curves as is
often seen in the psychology literature. The sigmoid shape occurs only for
problems containing hints. The shape for end-of-chapter problems that do not
contain any hints tends to be linear with more students falling into the first
(quick) and third (late) regions. Generally only about 45% of the students
finish within 2 min to 2 hours. Certain best-fit functions (within 2 min to 2
hours) seem to be a feature of the problem regardless of whether that problem
is done before or after a related problem. The group that does a problem second
having solved a related problem first has an advantage in time over the group
that solves the same problem first in most related problem-pairs. This
difference is seen as measured by the shift in peaks of the gradient curves.
The advantage (reduction) in median time to solution is as high as 35% with an
average of about 12%. This shows evidence of learning from the first problem.
PERC 2004 Organizing Committee |
|
N. Sanjay Rebello | |
Department of Physics | |
116 Cardwell Hall | |
Kansas State University | |
Manhattan, KS 66506-2601 | |
(785) 532-1539 office | (785) 532-6806 fax |
srebello@phys.ksu.edu | |
Rachel E. Scherr | |
Department of Physics | |
University of Maryland | |
082 Regents Drive | |
College Park, MD 20742-4111 | |
(301) 405-6179 office | (301) 314-9531 fax |
rescherr@physics.umd.edu | |
Michael C. Wittmann | |
Department of Physics & Astronomy | |
5709 Bennett Hall | |
University of Maine | |
Orono, ME 04469-5709 | |
(207) 581-1237 office | (207) 581-3410 fax |
wittmann@umit.maine.edu |